Study Shows Group Work Can Harm Memory

Sep 13, 2016 by News Staff

A new study published in the Psychological Bulletin reveals that collaborating in a group to remember information is harmful.

Group work can harm memory, researchers say.

Group work can harm memory, researchers say.

Collaborative remembering is important as it is used in a number of different everyday settings.

In the workplace, interview panels jointly recall candidates’ answers before deciding who to employ. In the courtroom, jurors work together to recall trial evidence prior to reaching a verdict. In schools and universities, students work together to revise course content prior to exams.

The new study, by Dr. Craig Thorley from the University of Liverpool and Dr. Stéphanie Marion from the University of Ontario, statistically analyzed 64 earlier collaborative remembering studies and found that groups recall less than their individual members would if working alone.

The researchers compared the recall of collaborative groups to the pooled recall of an equivalent number of individuals.

For example, if a collaborative group consisted of four people, their recall was compared to that of four individuals who worked alone but whose recall was combined.

Collaborative group recall was consistently lower than pooled individual recall. This effect is known as collaborative inhibition.

The study suggests collaborative inhibition occurs as group members disrupt each other’s retrieval strategies when recalling together.

“Collaborative group members develop their own preferred retrieval strategies for recalling information,” Dr. Thorley said.

“For example, Person A may prefer to recall information in the order it was learned but Person B may prefer to recall it in the reverse order. Importantly, recall is greatest when people can use their own preferred retrieval strategies.”

“During collaboration, members hear each other recall information using competing retrieval strategies and their preferred strategies become disrupted,” he said.

“This results in each group member underperforming and the group as a whole suffers. Individuals who work alone can use their preferred retrieval strategies without this disruption so recall more.”

Several factors were also found to influence the extent to which collaborative inhibition occurs.

One of these findings was collaboration is more harmful to larger groups than smaller groups. Another was that friends and family members are more effective at working together than strangers.

“Smaller groups perform better than larger groups as they contain fewer competing (disruptive) retrieval strategies,” Dr. Thorley said.

“Friends and family members perform better than strangers as they tend to develop complementary (and not competing) retrieval strategies.”

The team also found that collaborative remembering boosts later individual learning: people who previously recall in a group remember more than those who do not.

“We believe that this occurs as working in a group means people are re-exposed to things they may have forgotten and this boosts their memory later on,” Dr. Marion explained.

“One of the important consequences of this is that it suggests getting people to work together to remember something (e.g., students revising together) is beneficial for individual learning.”

_____

Stéphanie B. Marion & Craig Thorley. A Meta-Analytic Review of Collaborative Inhibition and Postcollaborative Memory: Testing the Predictions of the Retrieval Strategy Disruption Hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, published online September 12, 2016; doi: 10.1037/bul0000071

Share This Page